Sunday, 30 April 2017

Love is in the air - aerial courtship displays in the bird world.

Aerial courtship displays are fairly common throughout the avian world, however relatively little is known about the complexities and functions of these displays (Arroyo et al. 2013). Just as morphological characteristics can be used by females to assess the quality of a prospective male, so too can behavioural displays (Clark 2009). Some behavioural displays may serve as an honest indication of overall condition and may become elaborate and exaggerated due to directional selection caused by female preference (Arroyo et al. 2013, Clark 2009). 

The Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) is just one of many species of birds that perform aerial courtship displays, https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=koWpTZ8Jo-s. Viewed on 30th April 2017.

Male Anna's Hummingbirds (Calypte anna) perform spectacular aerial dives as a component of their courtship behaviour, in which they can reach speeds of up to 385 body lengths per second - achieving the highest known length-specific velocity of any vertebrate (Clark 2009). Males ascend to a height of approximately 30m before diving head first and swooping over a watching female (Clark & Feo 2008). As they perform this incredibly fast dive they emit a brief, loud squeak or chirp produced by air passing over their tail feathers (Clark & Feo 2008). It is a puzzling display as the males go to great lengths to produce this sound that requires extremely high velocities, but are capable of singing at the same frequency (Clark & Feo 2008). So why do they go to this effort to produce an accoustically similar sound mechanically when they can produce it vocally??? Another unanswered question!!!

The male Anna's Hummingbird (Calypte anna) achieves the highest known length-specific velocity of any vertebrate. Image sourced from http://animalia-life.club/birds/annas-hummingbird.html. Viewed on 28th April 2017.

The male Anna's Hummingbird (Calypte anna) performing his incredible aerial display. Listen and watch closely or you just might miss it! https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7Rm5RfuexGU. Viewed on 29th April 2017.

Raptors are well known for their spectacular aerial displays (Arroyo et al. 2013). In one species of raptor, the Montagu's Harrier (Circus pygargus), males perform frequent "sky-dancing" displays  leading up to the breeding season, in which they ascend before rapidly plunging downward while performing twists, undulations, loops and display calls (Simmons 1988). The timing of these displays suggests that they may well be used in the process of mate appraisal and selection (Arroyo et al. 2013, Simmons 1988). The "aerial display hypothesis" in which aerial agility and hence small size is favoured by sexual selection, is also supported by this species in which males are lighter and more agile than females (Arroyo et al. 2013, Simmons 1988).

The striking looking male Montagu's Harrier (Circus pygargus) perform frequent "sky-dancing" displays  leading up to the breeding season. Photo by

These are just two examples of the many species of birds that perform aerial courtship displays. I have been lucky enough to witness some of these incredible aerial courtship displays first-hand, the most impressive of which would have to be that of the Red-tailed Tropicbird, although I found it difficult to learn anything about this in the scientific literature. The birds gracefully circle each other over the ocean in the period leading up to the breeding season - a beautiful display by a beautiful bird!!!

Red-tailed Tropicbirds (Phaethon rubricauda) performing an aerial courtship display. Photo by Paul Mckenzie, http://www.wildencounters.net/2012/06/midway-atoll-april-2012/. Viewed on 29th April 2017.

References

Arroyo, B., Mougeot, F. & Bretagnolle, V. 2013, "Characteristics and sexual functions of sky-dancing displays in a semi-colonial raptor, the montagu's harrier (Circus pygargus)", Journal of Raptor Research, vol. 47, no. 2, pp. 185-196
 
Clark, C.J. 2009, "Courtship dives of Anna's hummingbird offer insights into flight performance limits", Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 276, no. 1670, pp. 3047-3052.

Clark, C.J. & Feo, T.J. 2008, "The Anna's hummingbird chirps with its tail: a new mechanism of sonation in birds", Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 275, no. 1637, pp. 955-962.

Simmons, R. 1988, "Honest advertising, sexual selection, courtship displays, and body condition of polygynous male harriers", The Auk, vol. 105, no. 2, pp. 303-307.

Friday, 21 April 2017

Olfactory cues in seabirds as a driver of sexual selection.

When we think of sexual selection in birds we usually think of colour, song and their associated displays, but what about scent? A functional sense of smell has been exhibited by every bird that has been tested, yet the study of chemical signalling in birds has been historically neglected (Hagelin et al. 2003, Roper 1999). It has now been shown that some species of seabirds display olfactory discrimination capabilities, suggesting that scent may provide valuable information as to the quality of prospective mates and also be used as an inbreeding avoidance mechanism (Mardon & Bonadonna 2009). 

The Blue Petrel (Halobaena caerulea) has been shown to prefer the odour of its mate and other conspecifics than that of itself (Mardon & Bonadonna 2009). This odour recognition and preference could serve many functions but it has been suggested that it may be used to facilitate genetically based mate choice (Mardon & Bonadonna 2009, Zelano & Edwards 2002). Olfactory signals from urine or body odours have been associated with certain genotypes in many species of vertebrates, meaning that chemical signalling may be also playing a role in mediating mate choice in birds (Mardon & Bonadonna 2009, Singh 2001).

https://oceanwide-4579.kxcdn.com/uploads/media-dynamic/cache/jpg_optimize/uploads/media/default/0001/08/thumb_7459_default_1600.jpeg
The Blue Petrel (Halobaena caerulea) may be using olfactory cues to facilitate genetically based mate choice. Image sourced from https://oceanwide-expeditions.com/to-do/wildlife/blue-petrel. Viewed on 18th April 2017.


The Crested Auklet (Aethia cristatella) emits a tangerine-scented odour during the breeding season that is thought to be used as a method of chemical communication (Hagelin et al. 2003). Throughout the courtship period both sexes emit this odour and frequently engage in 'ruff sniff' displays where individuals rub their beaks in the nape feathers of display partners, the region where the tangerine odour is the strongest (Jones & Hunter 1993). The fact that this scent is only produced, and 'ruff sniff' display only occurs in the breeding season, provides strong evidence that birds are using  olfactory cues in the process of sexual selection (Hagelin et al. 2003).

http://www.hbw.com/sites/default/files/styles/ibc_1k/public/ibc/p/crested_auklet__aethia_cristatella_talan_russia_june_24_2008_16_1024.jpg?itok=P43m_fPD
Crested Auklets (Aethia cristatella) engaged in a 'ruff sniff' display where individuals rub their beaks in the nape feathers of display partners, the region where the tangerine odour emitted is the strongest. Photo by Lars Petersson http://www.hbw.com/ibc/species/54074/photos. Viewed on 18th April 2017.


So next time you admire the striking colouration of a bird, or listen to a beautiful melody of birdsong, consider that there is often more to birds than meets the eye or ear. I have shown you throughout the blog so far how more well known and studied visual and accoustic-based cues can often be associated with sexual selection. These alone result in a huge variety of intriguing and incredible morphological and behavioural traits, but with olfactory cues thrown into the mix, yet another layer of complexity is added to the wonderful world of birds!



References

Hagelin, J.C., Jones, I.L. & Rasmussen L.E.L. 2003, "A tangerine-scented social odour in a monogamous seabird", Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 270, no. 1522, pp. 1323-1329.

Jones, I.L. & Hunter, F.M. 1993, "Mutual sexual selection in a monogamous seabird", Nature, vol. 362, no. 6417, pp. 238-239.

Mardon, J. & Bonadonna, F. 2009, "Atypical homing or self-odour avoidance? Blue Petrels (Halobaena caerulea) are attracted to their mate's odour but avoid their own", Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, vol. 63, no. 4, pp. 537-542.

Roper, T.J. 1999. "Olfaction in birds", Advances in the Study of Behavior, vol. 28, pp. 247-247.

Singh, P. 2001, "Chemosensation and genetic individuality", Reproduction, vol. 121, no. 4, pp. 529-539.

Zelano, B. & Edwards, S.V. 2002, "An MHC component to kin recognition and mate choice in birds: predictions, progress, and prospects", The American naturalist, vol. 160 Suppl 6, no. 6, pp. S225. 

Saturday, 15 April 2017

Vocal mimicry: a result of sexual selection???

One of the earliest memories I have of developing a love of birds was when I saw and heard a Superb Lyrebird (Menura novaehollandiae) for the first time. Growing up on the South Coast of NSW I was lucky enough to be able to venture into their habitat and witness first hand their incredible antics. Watching a lyrebird perfectly mimic the sound of a Laughing Kookaburra (Dacelo novaeguineae) blew my mind and I have since heard them while camping mimic the sound of campers banging in tent pegs. I have also been fooled and amazed when I heard what I thought was the unmistakable call of a Whistling Kite (Haliastur sphenurus) coming from a mango tree in Kakadu National Park, only to discover it was emanating from a Great Bowerbird (Chlamydera nuchalis). It is not just lyrebirds and bowerbirds that mimic other birds and environmental noises, it is suggested that 15-20% of passerines incorporate heterospecific elements into their songs (Zann & Dunstan 2008).

The male Superb Lyrebird (Menura novaehollandiae) has possibly the greatest song repertoire in the world. His ability to to accurately mimic up to 20 different species of birds along with many other environmental noises is truly amazing https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VjE0Kdfos4Y. Viewed on 12th April 2017.


So why do birds vocally mimic other bird species? The short answer is that we still don't really know. There have been many theories put forward that have tried to answer this complex question, one of which is that female preference for song complexity has resulted in mimicry to enlarge the male's song repertoire (Zann & Dunstan 2008). Research has since shown that in some species of birds, accuracy of vocal mimicry and size of repertoire in males can predict mating success better than other display traits that have been determined as important for male mating success (Coleman et al. 2007). The fact that mimicry is a learned behaviour requiring the modification of vocal motor patterns (Zollinger & Suthers 2004) has prompted the idea that mimicry accuracy may provide females with information as to the male's past (Nowicki et al. 2002) and present (Garamszegi 2005) condition and serve as an honest indicator of physiological performance (Coleman et al. 2007).

https://c1.staticflickr.com/5/4103/5024267447_efe331d29f_b.jpg
The Great Bowerbird (Chlamydera nuchalis) is also an excellent mimic and has certainly fooled me more than once! It has been suggested that Great Bowerbirds mimic predators to protect their nest, however field evidence does not support this hypothesis. Image sourced from https://www.flickr.com/photos/21342600@N03/5024267447.  Viewed on 13th April 2017.

Researching vocal mimicry in birds has once again highlighted the many complexities embedded within the avian world we do not, and may never, fully understand. It seems to me that researchers are often at risk of trying to prove each other wrong when in fact given the sheer diversity and vastly different characteristics of birds, in some cases there may be as many answers to the same question as there are species of birds.



References


Coleman, S.W., Patricelli, G.L., Coyle, B., Siani, J. & Borgia, G. 2007, "Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays", Biology Letters, vol. 3, no. 5, pp. 463-466.


Garamszegi, L.Z. 2005, "Age-dependent health status and song characteristics in the barn swallow", Behavioral Ecology, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 580-591.

Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. & Peters, S. 2002, "Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the "nutritional stress hypothesis"", Journal of Comparative Physiology A, vol. 188, no. 11, pp. 1003-1014.

Zann, R. & Dunstan, E. 2008, "Mimetic song in superb lyrebirds: species mimicked and mimetic accuracy in different populations and age classes", Animal Behaviour, vol. 76, no. 3, pp. 1043-1054.

Zollinger, S.A. & Suthers, R.A. 2004, "Motor mechanisms of a vocal mimic: implications for birdsong production", Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 271, no. 1538, pp. 483-491.

Wednesday, 5 April 2017

The great frigatebird: a puzzling case of inbreeding and sexual selection.

Inbreeding, or the mating of more genetically similar individuals than the average relatedness of individuals in a population, has been shown to reduce fitness through the increase in frequency of  deleterious recessive alleles being expressed in offspring (Cohen & Dearborn 2004). Throughout the animal and plant world, strategies have been developed that decrease the likelihood of inbreeding and the negative effects it can produce (Darwin 1876, Charlesworth & Charlesworth 1987, Pusey & Wolf 1996). However, in certain populations of organisms including birds, it does occur due to limited dispersal ability, random mating processes with respect to relatedness and natal breeding site fidelity (Bohonak 1999, Cohen & Dearborn 2004). But is this the ONLY reason we see inbreeding occurring in bird populations?

The male great frigatebird (Fregata minor) shows off his inflated red gular pouch to attract a female, but is this really what the female is inspecting? Could this be used by the female as an indicator of genetic similarity? Image sourced from http://www.pelagicodyssey.ca/styled-3/page37/. Viewed on 3rd April, 2017.


The great frigatebird (Fregata minor) population of Tern Island in Hawaii has been shown to display high levels of inbreeding, but not due to any of the reasons mentioned above (Cohen & Dearborn 2004). Great frigatebird breeding ecology would make it improbable that inbreeding would occur, as the species does not suffer from any of the constraints that would "force"  genetically similar individuals to breed with each other - females aerially assess the displays of up to several hundred males, males can travel hundreds of kilometers to display to females on other islands and interisland genetic diversity is reasonably high (Dearborn & Ryan 2002, Dearborn et al. 2003). While great frigatebirds do show a degree of natal site fidelity, females have a broad range of males to choose from as the operational sex ratio is male biased at the time the sexes are pairing-up to breed (Dearborn et al. 2001). 

Great frigatebirds (Fregata minor) (foreground), and red-footed boobies (Sula sula) (background) both breed on Tern Island in Hawaii. Photo by P.D. Tillmam https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Great_frigatebirds_and_red-footed_boobies_at_Tern_Island.jpg. Viewed on 3rd April, 2017.


Despite these breeding behaviours and opportunities for females to pick genetically dissimilar males, inbreeding is widespread throughout the population (Cohen & Dearborn 2004). This would suggest that females are actively picking genetically similar males to breed with (Cohen & Dearborn 2004). Sexual imprinting, descibed by Irwin & Price (1999) as "the means by which a young bird learns species-specific characteristics that enables it to find a conspecific mate when adult", has been suggested as a possible mechanism for inbreeding in the great frigatebird, but there is still much uncertainty about the resulting fitness consequences and whether they are beneficial or detrimental (Cohen & Dearborn 2004). There would have to be an advantage associated with inbreeding in this case if the postulates of sexual selection apply wouldn't there??? A very interesting but still unanswered question!



References 

Bohonak, A.J. 1999, "Dispersal, gene flow, and population structure", The Quarterly Review of Biology, vol. 74, no. 1, pp. 21-45.

Charlesworth, D. & Charlesworth, B. 1987, "Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences", Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 237-268.

Cohen, L.B. & Dearborn, D.C. 2004, "Great frigatebirds, Fregata minor, choose mates that are genetically similar", Animal Behaviour, vol. 68, no. 5, pp. 1229-1236.

Darwin, C. 1876, The effects of cross and self fertilisation in the vegetable kingdom. J. Murray.

Dearborn, D.C., Anders, A. D. & Parker, P. G. 2001, "Sexual dimorphism, extrapair fertilizations, and operational sex ratio in great frigatebirds (Fregata minor)", Behavioral Ecology, vol. 12, no. 6, pp. 746-752.

Dearborn, D.C., Anders, A.D., Schreiber, E.A., Adams, R.M.M. & Mueller, U.G. 2003, "Inter‐island movements and population differentiation in a pelagic seabird", Molecular Ecology, vol. 12, no. 10, pp. 2835-2843. 

Dearborn, D. C. & Ryan, M. J. 2002, "A test of the Darwin–Fisher theory for the evolution of male secondary sexual traits in monogamous birds", Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 15, pp. 307–313.

Irwin, D.E. & Price, T. 1999, "Sexual imprinting, learning and speciation", Heredity, vol. 82, no. 4, pp. 347-354.

Pusey, A. & Wolf, M. 1996, "Inbreeding avoidance in animals", Trends in Ecology & Evolution, vol. 11, no. 5, pp. 201-206.


 

Thursday, 23 March 2017

Sexual dimorphism in birds: a complexity of sexual selection.

Why is it that in some species of birds males and females look so different you could easily mistake them for different species, while in others they look exactly the same? Evolutionary biologists have been looking for answers to this question since Darwin published The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (1871) (Dunn et al. 2001).

Extreme sexual dimorphism is displayed in the Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus) - the male (rear) being spectacularly ornamented, while the female (front) displays drab colouration and a smaller body size. Photo by Kimball Chen http://keaphotography.photoshelter.com/image/I0000F1Owdy4Zd9U. Viewed on 18th March 2017.

Mandarin Ducks (Aix galericulata) are another great example of sexual dimorphism in birds - the male (left) has striking ornamentation compared to the female (right). Image sourced from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Mandarin_duck. Viewed on 19th March 2017.

The general trend when it comes to sexual dimorphism is that males often display much brighter plumage and/or are larger in size than their female counterparts. Darwin (1871) and Wallace (1889) initially put this down to differences in social mating systems and paternal care, with polygamy causing the competitive sex to become larger and more ornate than the choosy sex and the sex that provides the parental care to develop more cryptic plumage (Owens & Hartley 1998). But if this is strictly true, why do we see socially monogamous species exhibiting biparental care that are highly dimorphic?


While size dimorphism displayed in some species of birds can be largely accounted for by the traditional idea of intrasexual competition, polygamy and differences in parental care (Darwin 1871, Owens & Hartley 1998, Wallace 1889), plumage dimorphism in socially monogamous birds is a more contentious issue. It has now been suggested that plumage dimorphism in socially monogamous birds may be due to extra-bond copulations (this is when despite having one partner whom each bird rears their young with, both males and females copulate with other individuals) (Owens & Hartley 1998, Webster et al. 2007). Molecular techniques have shown that up to 65% of socially monogamous birds display extra-bond fertilisation (Owens & Hartley 1998). If the extra-bond copulations that result in fertilisations are not random, which is almost certainly the case, then sexual selection should occur and favour traits displayed by males that are preferred by the promiscuous females (Owens & Hartley 1998, Webster et al. 2007).

The Superb Fairy Wren (Malurus cyaneus) is a great example of sexual dimorphism - the male's (left) vibrant blue plumage is strikingly different to the dull grey/brown plumage of the female (right). They are a socially monogamous bird but are known to be extremely promiscuous!. Image sourced from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Male_and_female_superb_fairy_wren_edit21.jpg. Viewed on 19th March 2017.

 Just to make things more complicated, support has been growing for the idea that Wallace (1889) originally favoured - that plumage colour dimorphism is driven by the parent who primarily cares for the young, in most cases the female Owens & Hartley 1998) - but not how you think! Research into how hormones control sexual dimorphism in birds has shown that in many species, the default plumage is actually the bright, showy plumage we expect to see in males (Owens & Hartley 1998). This means that plumage colour dimorphism, in at least some cases, is due to females actively trying to become more cryptic by suppressing the default bright, showy plumage (Owens & Hartley 1998). I never knew this and I think it is incredible!

The Eclectus Parrot (Eclectus roratus) displays a unique form of sexual dimorphism in that the female (right) is actually brighter and more conspicuous than the male (left). This is due to different selection pressures acting on each of the sexes (Heinsohn et al. 2005). Image sourced from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Eclectus_parrot. Viewed on 20th March 2017.

References

Darwin, C. 1871, The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: Murray.

Dunn, P.O., Whittingham, L.A. & Pitcher, T.E. 2001, "Mating systems, sperm competition, and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in birds", Evolution, vol. 55, no. 1, pp. 161-17.

Heinsohn, R., Legge, S. & Endler, J.A. 2005, "Extreme Reversed Sexual Dichromatism in a Bird without Sex Role Reversal", Science, vol. 309, no. 5734, pp. 617-619.  

Owens, I.P.F. & Hartley, I.R. 1998, "Sexual dimorphism in birds: why are there so many different forms of dimorphism?", Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 265, no. 1394, pp. 397-407. 

Wallace, A.R. 1889, Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection, with Some of the Applications. Macmillan.
 
Webster, M.S., Tarvin, K.A., Tuttle, E.M. & Pruett-Jones, S. 2007, "Promiscuity drives sexual selection in a socially monogamous bird", Evolution, vol. 61, no. 9, pp. 2205-2211.



Wednesday, 15 March 2017

Bowerbirds: The greatest display of sexual selection in the world?????

I cannot complete this blog without having a post dedicated entirely to bowerbirds. Bowerbirds are exceptional in that they display a multifaceted and extremely entertaining approach to sexual display. Bower building and ornamentation is what they are most famous for, but dancing and strange vocal displays that often incorporate mimicry are also utilised to tempt females into reproduction (Marshall 1954)

Bowers
The first and most important part of the sexual display exhibited by male bowerbirds, is the ability to build a bower (where courtship and copulation take place) of sufficient quality to attract a prospective mate (Borgia 1985, Marshall 1954). Males are highly territorial and frequently destroy and steal decorations from other bowers, so the ability of a male to build, defend and display in a highly decorated bower is thought to provide the female information as to the overall quality of the male (Borgia 1985, Robson et al. 2005). It has been shown that  females are more attracted to males who have high quality bowers, and it is these males who account for the majority of matings (Borgia 1985).

The male Fawn-breasted Bowerbird (Chlamydera cerviniventris) creates his 1-2 foot long tunnel with an open top, known as an avenue-style bower, decorated with bright green leaves. Image sourced from http://animals.sandiegozoo.org/animals/bowerbird. Viewed on 11th March 2017.
The bower of a Great Bowerbird (Chlamydera nuchalis) (the largest of the bowerbirds) from Northern Australia is decorated primarily with white and grey objects such as snail shells and pebbles. Photo by Peter Rowland http://australiaswildlife.com/2016/05/bowerbirds-of-australia/. Viewed on 12th March 2017.
The incredible bower of the male Satin Bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus). Image sourced from: http://www.viralforest.com/bower-bird/. Viewed on 12th March 2017.

The 'totem' bower of the Golden Bowerbird (Prionodura newtoniana) of Far North Queensland can be up to 2m high and is decorated with lichen and freshly plucked flowers. Photo by Tim Laman, http://timlaman.photoshelter.com/image/I00004AVpNp1wFWk. Viewed on 11th March 2017.
up to 2 m high, decorated with lichen and freshly plucked flower - See more at: http://www.australianwildlife.org/wildlife/golden-bowerbird.aspx#sthash.jTZwvNR5.dpuf
up to 2 m high, decorated with lichen and freshly plucked flower - See more at: http://www.australianwildlife.org/wildlife/golden-bowerbird.aspx#sthash.jTZwvNR5.dpuf
up to 2 m high, decorated with lichen and freshly plucked flower - See more at: http://www.australianwildlife.org/wildlife/golden-bowerbird.aspx#sthash.jTZwvNR5.dpuf
up to 2 m high, decorated with lichen and freshly plucked flower - See more at: http://www.australianwildlife.org/wildlife/golden-bowerbird.aspx#sthash.jTZwvNR5.dpuf


Perhaps the most impressive of all bowers, the giant bower of the Vogelkop Bowerbird (Amblyornis inornata) is a true masterpiece! Make sure you watch the video to find out more and have your mind blown. Photo by Ingo Arndt, http://www.audubon.org/news/see-rare-extraordinary-and-natural-photos-natgeos-new-book. Viewed on 10th March 2017.
Display
If the male manages to get past the first stage of impressing the female with his architectural and adornment skills, he then gets the chance to perform his display to 'seal the deal' so to speak, and copulate. Research has shown that the intensity and aggression of the male Satin Bowerbird's (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) display preferred by the female, varies with age and experience and that a successful courtship may be the result of the male reading the female's reaction to his display and adjusting it accordingly (Coleman et al. 2004, Patricelli 2004, Walsh et al. 2002) . This theory of variable female preference has been implicated as a possible driver of the evolution of multifaceted male displays (Coleman et al. 2004, Patricelli 2004, Walsh et al. 2002).


Bowerbirds and the behaviour they display are extremely diverse and what I have shown you here is just a glimpse into their complexity. As Darwin predicted, all evidence points towards a combination of female choosiness and male competition resulting in the exceptional behaviours and traits we see in bowerbirds. I have added some videos below that sum up why I love bowerbirds so much. Where I grew up there are lots of Satin Bowerbirds and the first time I found one of their bowers I was absolutely amazed. I can describe all the incredible things they do, but it is only fully appreciated when you see what they do. Enjoy!
       The male satin bowerbird's (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) display https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_H9TyXiXM2k. Viewed on 11th March 2017.

The male vogelkop bowerbird (Amblyornis inornata) shows us why he is so incredible! https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=E1zmfTr2d4c. Viewed on 10th March 2017.
 

     The flame bowerbird's (Sericulus aureus) bizarre and mesmerising display https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1XkPeN3AWIE. Viewed on 11th March 2017.

References

Borgia, G. 1985, "Bower quality, number of decorations and mating success of male satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus): an experimental analysis", Animal Behaviour, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 266-271.

Coleman, S.W., Patricelli, G.L. & Borgia, G. 2004, "Variable female preferences drive complex male displays", Nature, vol. 428, no. 6984, pp. 742-745.

Marshall, A.J. 1954, Bower-birds: their displays and breeding cycles, Clarendon Press, Oxford.

Robson, T.E., Goldizen, A.W. & Green, D.J. 2005, "The multiple signals assessed by female satin bowerbirds: could they be used to narrow down females' choices of mates?", Biology Letters, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 264-267

Patricelli, G.L. 2004, "Female signals enhance the efficiency of mate assessment in satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus)", Behavioral Ecology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 297-304.

Walsh, G., Borgia, G., Patricelli, G.L. & Uy, J.A.C. 2002, "Sexual selection Male displays adjusted to female's response", Nature, vol. 415, no. 6869, pp. 279-280.